Trends in Neurosciences
Volume 36, Issue 8, August 2013, Pages 489-496
Journal home page for Trends in Neurosciences

Review
Toward a cross-species understanding of empathy

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2013.04.009Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Signs of empathy have been observed in many mammals, including laboratory rodents.

  • ‘Primal empathy’ utilizes the seven basic emotional systems of the mammalian brain.

  • Affective neuroscience approaches can elucidate the underlying brain substrates.

  • Continued study of primal empathy in rodents will benefit mental health practices.

  • A neuro-evolutionary paradigm can illuminate how empathy is expressed in humans.

Although signs of empathy have now been well documented in non-human primates, only during the past few years have systematic observations suggested that a primal form of empathy exists in rodents. Thus, the study of empathy in animals has started in earnest. Here we review recent studies indicating that rodents are able to share states of fear, and highlight how affective neuroscience approaches to the study of primary-process emotional systems can help to delineate how primal empathy is constituted in mammalian brains. Cross-species evolutionary approaches to understanding the neural circuitry of emotional ‘contagion’ or ‘resonance’ between nearby animals, together with the underlying neurochemistries, may help to clarify the origins of human empathy.

Introduction

Empathy reflects the capacity of one animal to experience the emotional feelings of another, a process with many cognitive refinements in humans. Thus, investigators commonly distinguish between emotional and cognitive forms of empathy (see below) 1, 2. Studies of empathy make up a relatively new subdiscipline in neuroscience, with human brain imaging providing many correlates of relevant, higher psychological functions 3, 4, 5. Neuroscience research on empathy in other animals has lagged far behind, but simplified animal behavior models based on emotional contagion, the presumed foundations of empathy, have been developed (Figure 1) [6]. Our goal here is to summarize such novel empirical approaches for studying empathy in laboratory rats and mice, and to highlight an integrated neuro-evolutionary strategy for understanding human empathy.

Before proceeding, we consider the meteoric rise of neuro-empathy studies during the past few decades. The study of empathy was sparse in the biologically-oriented sciences of the 20th century until E.O. Wilson's Sociobiology (1975), where constructs such as kin selection and reciprocal altruism were seen as major evolutionary explanations for individuals behaving unselfishly, even ‘altruistically’, toward others, provided that such behaviors supported the survival of one's own genes [7]. Indeed, in Descent of Man, Darwin suggested that ‘We are thus impelled to relieve the sufferings of another, in order that our own painful feelings may at the same time be relieved’ and ‘those communities which included the greatest number of the most sympathetic members would flourish best, and rear the greatest number of offspring’ ([8], p. 88). Thus, inspired by writings of philosophers such as John Stuart Mill and Adam Smith, together with American social psychologists such as William McDougall [9] and Russian evolutionist Pyotr Kropotkin [10], a prosocial perspective emerged in late 20th century suggesting that individuals might be constitutionally more cooperative and emotionally interdependent than previously considered.

By the late 1990s human brain imaging offered robust approaches for identifying brain regions aroused during emotional states, encouraging systematic neuropsychological studies of empathy 11, 12 that have now yielded diverse affective, cognitive, and social neuroscience perspectives 1, 13, 14, 15. Concurrently, primatologists recognized signs of empathic sensitivities 16, 17 and now neuroscientists, inspired by classic early behavioral studies 18, 19, 20, are fashioning reliable simplified models to study the evolutionary roots of empathy (Box 1 and Figure 1)

Section snippets

The vagaries of ‘empathic’ terminologies

The term ‘empathy’ continues to have a diverse as well as nebulous usage, with ‘sympathy’ and ‘compassion’ being perennial colloquialisms used to describe related phenomena. One must remember that the term is a recent contribution to the vernacular, emerging in the early 20th century from the Greek empatheia (from em- ‘in’ + pathos ‘feeling’) and translated into the German Einfühlung, namely ‘feeling into’, especially when humans aesthetically appreciate the beauty of art. The English version of

Evolutionary affective foundations of empathy: levels of analysis in the brain and mind

Clearly a detailed, constitutive understanding of the mechanisms of empathy must come from cross-species neuroscience. Given the many excellent reviews covering correlative human brain imaging of empathy 3, 11, 24, 25, 26, 27, we focus here on the primal emotional foundations of empathy in mammalian brains. The ‘primary-process’ emotional systems of the brain, which generate affective feelings (Box 2), are more accessible in animal models than in humans 23, 28. The interaction of primal

Beyond terminological and conceptual conundrums

A cross-species analysis readily synergizes with the original approach of Lipps, whereby empathy was characterized by how ‘the perception of an emotional gesture in another directly activates the same emotion in the perceiver, without any intervening labeling, associative or cognitive perspective-taking processes’ ([22], p. 2). As succinctly put by Hoffman [14], empathy is the process through which an individual generates an ‘affective response more appropriate to someone else's situation than

Top-down cognitive regulation of empathy

With maturation, guided by the affective qualities of childhood [54], humans develop the thought-related cognitive functions that vary considerably among individuals, providing diverse top-down control over social relationships 33, 55. Obviously, much having to do with empathy among humans is cognitive and elaborated by higher brain functions (e.g., sense of fairness, sympathetic perspective-taking, and compassion). However, we need to consider how those higher functions developmentally arise

Primary-process empathy

In its most basic form, empathy may be an inherent property of primal emotional systems, reflecting the fact that there is perceptually induced resonance of the same affective states in nearby animals. This may take its most poignant form in the capacity of mothers to intrinsically understand the emotional feelings of their infants. For instance, PANIC networks engender separation calls to signal psychological distress (probably a form of psychic pain evolving from pre-existing systems that

Secondary-process modulation and parsing of primal empathy

It is not clear that secondary learning and memory processes contribute anything unique to the spectrum of primal empathic responses beyond parsing those responses in terms of space, time, and intensity. This level of brain processing appears to be completely unconscious 32, 48, with well-established circuits for emotional learning (especially fear-conditioning) operating without the addition of anything new in terms of subjective emotional experiences. Learning can both intensify and moderate

Tertiary processes: the social emotions writ large

These highest levels of psychological processing are best studied in humans. There is a vast literature on human empathy, with insufficient space here to add much of significance. However, this is where research on the neurochemistries of social bonding – first opioids and subsequently oxytocin – both of which reduce separation-induced PANIC robustly (reviewed in 23, 48), may engender differential predictions about empathy – opioids may reduce empathy by dampening experienced social pain

The social foundations of empathy

Modern neuroscience is beginning to provide tools to clarify how the evolutionary sources of empathy are deeply grounded in fundamental brain processes – not only in our capacity to experience physical pains but also in the emotional networks that make us socially interdependent creatures. Although empathy has different connotations for different people, most agree that it is deeply emotional and pro-social – whereby one responds to the bodily needs, pains, and psychological losses of others

Acknowledgments

J.B.P. was supported by a National Institutes of Health (NIH) training grant (F32 MH096475) during the writing of this paper.

References (93)

  • A. Nowak

    Social modulation in the extinction of aversive memories

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2013)
  • A.K. Bruchey

    Fear conditioning by-proxy: social transmission of fear during memory retreival

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2010)
  • M. Kavaliers

    Recognition and avoidance of the odors of parasitized conspecifics and predators: differential genomic correlates

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2005)
  • R. Feldman

    Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans

    Horm. Behav.

    (2012)
  • E.E. Nelson et al.

    Brain substrates of infant–mother attachment; contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (1998)
  • O. Weisman

    Oxytocin administration to parent enhances infant physiological and behavioral readiness for social engagement

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2012)
  • J. Panksepp

    Primary process affects and brain oxytocin

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • G. Northoff et al.

    The trans-species concept of self and the subcortical–cortical midline system

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2008)
  • M.H. Davis

    Empathy: A Social Psychological Approach

    (1994)
  • B.C. Bernhardt et al.

    The neural basis of empathy

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • T. Singer et al.

    The social neuroscience of empathy

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2009)
  • J. Panksepp

    Altruism, neurobiology

  • C. Darwin

    The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex

    (1888)
  • S.D. Preston et al.

    The many faces of empathy: parsing empathic phenomena through a proximate, dynamic-systems view of representing the other in the self

    Emot. Rev.

    (2011)
  • L.A. Dugatkin

    The Prince of Evolution: Peter Kropotkin's Adventures in Science and Politics

    (2011)
  • N.I. Eisenberger

    Prosocial development

  • M.L. Hoffman

    The contribution of empathy to justice and moral judgment

  • C. Zahn-Waxler

    Psychophysiological correlates of empathy and prosocial behaviors in preschool children with behavior problems

    Dev. Psychopathol.

    (1995)
  • S.D. Preston et al.

    Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (2002)
  • F. de Waal

    Primates and Philosophers: How Morality Evolved

    (2006)
  • R.M. Church

    Emotional reactions of rats to the pain of others

    J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.

    (1959)
  • G.E. Rice et al.

    Altruism in the albino rat

    J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.

    (1962)
  • D. Riess

    Vicarious conditioned acceleration: successful observational learning of an aversive Pavlovian stimulus contingency

    J. Exp. Anal. Behav.

    (1972)
  • E.B. Titchener

    Lectures on the Experimental Psychology of Thought-Processes

    (1909)
  • T. Lipps

    Einfühlung, innere Nachahmung, und Organepfindungen

    Arch. Psychol.

    (1903)
  • J. Panksepp

    Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions

    (1998/2005)
  • J. Decety

    The neuroevolution of empathy

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2011)
  • C. McCall et al.

    The animal and human neuroendocrinology of social cognition, motivation and behavior

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • J. Panksepp

    How Primary-process emotional systems guide child development: ancestral regulators of human happiness, thriving, and suffering

  • J. Panksepp

    Empathy and the laws of affect

    Science

    (2011)
  • F.B.M. de Waal

    The antiquity of empathy

    Science

    (2012)
  • M. Solms et al.

    The ‘Id’ knows more than the ‘Ego’ admits: neuropsychoanalytic and primal consciousness perspectives on the interface between affective and cognitive neuroscience

    Brain Sci.

    (2012)
  • J. Panksepp

    Cross-species affective neuroscience decoding of the primal affective experiences of humans and related animals

    PLoS ONE

    (2011)
  • Cited by (204)

    • The intersection of empathy and addiction

      2023, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text